CAT Exam  >  CAT Tests  >  CAT Practice Test - 6 - CAT MCQ

CAT Practice Test - 6 - CAT MCQ


Test Description

30 Questions MCQ Test - CAT Practice Test - 6

CAT Practice Test - 6 for CAT 2024 is part of CAT preparation. The CAT Practice Test - 6 questions and answers have been prepared according to the CAT exam syllabus.The CAT Practice Test - 6 MCQs are made for CAT 2024 Exam. Find important definitions, questions, notes, meanings, examples, exercises, MCQs and online tests for CAT Practice Test - 6 below.
Solutions of CAT Practice Test - 6 questions in English are available as part of our course for CAT & CAT Practice Test - 6 solutions in Hindi for CAT course. Download more important topics, notes, lectures and mock test series for CAT Exam by signing up for free. Attempt CAT Practice Test - 6 | 76 questions in 120 minutes | Mock test for CAT preparation | Free important questions MCQ to study for CAT Exam | Download free PDF with solutions
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 1

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Wherever I turn, the popular media, scientists and even fellow philosophers are telling me that I’m a machine or a beast. My ethics can be illuminated by the behavior of termites. My brain is a sloppy computer with a flicker of consciousness and the illusion of free will. I’m anything but human. While it would take more time and space than I have here to refute these views, I’d like to suggest why I stubbornly continue to believe that I’m a human being — something more than other animals, and essentially more than any computer.
Let’s begin with ethics. Many organisms carry genes that promote behavior that benefits other organisms. The classic example is ants: every individual insect is ready to sacrifice itself for the colony. As Edward O. Wilson explained in a recent essay, some biologists account for self-sacrificing behavior by the theory of kin selection, while Wilson and others favor group selection. Selection also operates between individuals: “within groups selfish individuals beat altruistic individuals, but groups of altruists beat groups of selfish individuals. Or, risking oversimplification, individual selection promoted sin, while group selection promoted virtue.” Wilson is cautious here, but some “evolutionary ethicists” don’t hesitate to claim that all we need in order to understand human virtue is the right explanation — whatever it may be — of how altruistic behavior evolved.
I have no beef with entomology or evolution, but I refuse to admit that they teach me much about ethics. Consider the fact that human action ranges to the extremes. People can perform extraordinary acts of altruism, including kindness toward other species — or they can utterly fail to be altruistic, even toward their own children. So whatever tendencies we may have inherited leave ample room for variation; our choices will determine which end of the spectrum we approach. This is where ethical discourse comes in — not in explaining how we’re “built,” but in deliberating on our own future acts. Should I cheat on this test? Should I give this stranger a ride? Knowing how my selfish and altruistic feelings evolved doesn’t help me decide at all. Most, though not all, moral codes advise me to cultivate altruism. But since the human race has evolved to be capable of a wide range of both selfish and altruistic behavior, there is no reason to say that altruism is superior to selfishness in any biological sense.
In fact, the very idea of an “ought” is foreign to evolutionary theory. It makes no sense for a biologist to say that some particular animal should be more cooperative, much less to claim that an entire species ought to aim for some degree of altruism. If we decide that we should neither “dissolve society” through extreme selfishness, as Wilson puts it, nor become “angelic robots” like ants, we are making an ethical judgment, not a biological one. Likewise, from a biological perspective it has no significance to claim that I should be more generous than I usually am, or that a tyrant ought to be deposed and tried. In short, a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless.
Some might draw the self-contradictory conclusion that we ought to drop the word “ought.” I prefer to conclude that ants are anything but human. They may feel pain and pleasure, which are the first glimmerings of purpose, but they’re nowhere near human (much less angelic) goodness. Whether we’re talking about ants, wolves, or naked mole rats, cooperative animal behavior is not human virtue. Any understanding of human good and evil has to deal with phenomena that biology ignores or tries to explain away — such as decency, self-respect, integrity, honor, loyalty or justice. These matters are debatable and uncertain — maybe permanently so. But that’s a far cry from being meaningless.

Q. The author of the passage:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 1

In the given case, the author of the passage makes a very assertion: Biological explanations are not sufficient to explain human behavior. This explanation is mirrored by option 3.

► Option 1 is unclear as it refers to 'some biological theories'. The author makes no such distinction.

► Option 2 is clearly not related to the passage as the author does not such thing.

► Option 4 is incorrect as the author is not hoodwinking (deceiving) the reader.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 2

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Wherever I turn, the popular media, scientists and even fellow philosophers are telling me that I’m a machine or a beast. My ethics can be illuminated by the behavior of termites. My brain is a sloppy computer with a flicker of consciousness and the illusion of free will. I’m anything but human. While it would take more time and space than I have here to refute these views, I’d like to suggest why I stubbornly continue to believe that I’m a human being — something more than other animals, and essentially more than any computer.
Let’s begin with ethics. Many organisms carry genes that promote behavior that benefits other organisms. The classic example is ants: every individual insect is ready to sacrifice itself for the colony. As Edward O. Wilson explained in a recent essay, some biologists account for self-sacrificing behavior by the theory of kin selection, while Wilson and others favor group selection. Selection also operates between individuals: “within groups selfish individuals beat altruistic individuals, but groups of altruists beat groups of selfish individuals. Or, risking oversimplification, individual selection promoted sin, while group selection promoted virtue.” Wilson is cautious here, but some “evolutionary ethicists” don’t hesitate to claim that all we need in order to understand human virtue is the right explanation — whatever it may be — of how altruistic behavior evolved.
I have no beef with entomology or evolution, but I refuse to admit that they teach me much about ethics. Consider the fact that human action ranges to the extremes. People can perform extraordinary acts of altruism, including kindness toward other species — or they can utterly fail to be altruistic, even toward their own children. So whatever tendencies we may have inherited leave ample room for variation; our choices will determine which end of the spectrum we approach. This is where ethical discourse comes in — not in explaining how we’re “built,” but in deliberating on our own future acts. Should I cheat on this test? Should I give this stranger a ride? Knowing how my selfish and altruistic feelings evolved doesn’t help me decide at all. Most, though not all, moral codes advise me to cultivate altruism. But since the human race has evolved to be capable of a wide range of both selfish and altruistic behavior, there is no reason to say that altruism is superior to selfishness in any biological sense.
In fact, the very idea of an “ought” is foreign to evolutionary theory. It makes no sense for a biologist to say that some particular animal should be more cooperative, much less to claim that an entire species ought to aim for some degree of altruism. If we decide that we should neither “dissolve society” through extreme selfishness, as Wilson puts it, nor become “angelic robots” like ants, we are making an ethical judgment, not a biological one. Likewise, from a biological perspective it has no significance to claim that I should be more generous than I usually am, or that a tyrant ought to be deposed and tried. In short, a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless.
Some might draw the self-contradictory conclusion that we ought to drop the word “ought.” I prefer to conclude that ants are anything but human. They may feel pain and pleasure, which are the first glimmerings of purpose, but they’re nowhere near human (much less angelic) goodness. Whether we’re talking about ants, wolves, or naked mole rats, cooperative animal behavior is not human virtue. Any understanding of human good and evil has to deal with phenomena that biology ignores or tries to explain away — such as decency, self-respect, integrity, honor, loyalty or justice. These matters are debatable and uncertain — maybe permanently so. But that’s a far cry from being meaningless.

Q. The author of the passage is most likely to agree with the statement:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 2

Answer to this question can be found in the lines: While it would take more time and space than I have here to refute these views, I’d like to suggest why I stubbornly continue to believe that I’m a human being — something more than other animals, and essentially more than any computer.

► These lines clearly indicate option 3 as the correct answer.

The idioms whole heap more means a great deal more.

1 Crore+ students have signed up on EduRev. Have you? Download the App
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 3

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Wherever I turn, the popular media, scientists and even fellow philosophers are telling me that I’m a machine or a beast. My ethics can be illuminated by the behavior of termites. My brain is a sloppy computer with a flicker of consciousness and the illusion of free will. I’m anything but human. While it would take more time and space than I have here to refute these views, I’d like to suggest why I stubbornly continue to believe that I’m a human being — something more than other animals, and essentially more than any computer.
Let’s begin with ethics. Many organisms carry genes that promote behavior that benefits other organisms. The classic example is ants: every individual insect is ready to sacrifice itself for the colony. As Edward O. Wilson explained in a recent essay, some biologists account for self-sacrificing behavior by the theory of kin selection, while Wilson and others favor group selection. Selection also operates between individuals: “within groups selfish individuals beat altruistic individuals, but groups of altruists beat groups of selfish individuals. Or, risking oversimplification, individual selection promoted sin, while group selection promoted virtue.” Wilson is cautious here, but some “evolutionary ethicists” don’t hesitate to claim that all we need in order to understand human virtue is the right explanation — whatever it may be — of how altruistic behavior evolved.
I have no beef with entomology or evolution, but I refuse to admit that they teach me much about ethics. Consider the fact that human action ranges to the extremes. People can perform extraordinary acts of altruism, including kindness toward other species — or they can utterly fail to be altruistic, even toward their own children. So whatever tendencies we may have inherited leave ample room for variation; our choices will determine which end of the spectrum we approach. This is where ethical discourse comes in — not in explaining how we’re “built,” but in deliberating on our own future acts. Should I cheat on this test? Should I give this stranger a ride? Knowing how my selfish and altruistic feelings evolved doesn’t help me decide at all. Most, though not all, moral codes advise me to cultivate altruism. But since the human race has evolved to be capable of a wide range of both selfish and altruistic behavior, there is no reason to say that altruism is superior to selfishness in any biological sense.
In fact, the very idea of an “ought” is foreign to evolutionary theory. It makes no sense for a biologist to say that some particular animal should be more cooperative, much less to claim that an entire species ought to aim for some degree of altruism. If we decide that we should neither “dissolve society” through extreme selfishness, as Wilson puts it, nor become “angelic robots” like ants, we are making an ethical judgment, not a biological one. Likewise, from a biological perspective it has no significance to claim that I should be more generous than I usually am, or that a tyrant ought to be deposed and tried. In short, a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless.
Some might draw the self-contradictory conclusion that we ought to drop the word “ought.” I prefer to conclude that ants are anything but human. They may feel pain and pleasure, which are the first glimmerings of purpose, but they’re nowhere near human (much less angelic) goodness. Whether we’re talking about ants, wolves, or naked mole rats, cooperative animal behavior is not human virtue. Any understanding of human good and evil has to deal with phenomena that biology ignores or tries to explain away — such as decency, self-respect, integrity, honor, loyalty or justice. These matters are debatable and uncertain — maybe permanently so. But that’s a far cry from being meaningless.

Q. What does the author mean when he says that “a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless”?​

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 3

Refer to the following lines to identify the answer: In fact, the very idea of an “ought” is foreign to evolutionary theory. It makes no sense for a biologist to say that some particular animal should be more cooperative, much less to claim that an entire species ought to aim for some degree of altruism. If we decide that we should neither “dissolve society” through extreme selfishness, as Wilson puts it, nor become “angelic robots” like ants, we are making an ethical judgment, not a biological one. Likewise, from a biological perspective it has no significance to claim that I should be more generous than I usually am, or that a tyrant ought to be deposed and tried. In short, a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless.

The author of the passage clearly expresses that evolution and ethics are not related to each other and evolution cannot be used to explain ethics and its principles.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 4

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Wherever I turn, the popular media, scientists and even fellow philosophers are telling me that I’m a machine or a beast. My ethics can be illuminated by the behavior of termites. My brain is a sloppy computer with a flicker of consciousness and the illusion of free will. I’m anything but human. While it would take more time and space than I have here to refute these views, I’d like to suggest why I stubbornly continue to believe that I’m a human being — something more than other animals, and essentially more than any computer.
Let’s begin with ethics. Many organisms carry genes that promote behavior that benefits other organisms. The classic example is ants: every individual insect is ready to sacrifice itself for the colony. As Edward O. Wilson explained in a recent essay, some biologists account for self-sacrificing behavior by the theory of kin selection, while Wilson and others favor group selection. Selection also operates between individuals: “within groups selfish individuals beat altruistic individuals, but groups of altruists beat groups of selfish individuals. Or, risking oversimplification, individual selection promoted sin, while group selection promoted virtue.” Wilson is cautious here, but some “evolutionary ethicists” don’t hesitate to claim that all we need in order to understand human virtue is the right explanation — whatever it may be — of how altruistic behavior evolved.
I have no beef with entomology or evolution, but I refuse to admit that they teach me much about ethics. Consider the fact that human action ranges to the extremes. People can perform extraordinary acts of altruism, including kindness toward other species — or they can utterly fail to be altruistic, even toward their own children. So whatever tendencies we may have inherited leave ample room for variation; our choices will determine which end of the spectrum we approach. This is where ethical discourse comes in — not in explaining how we’re “built,” but in deliberating on our own future acts. Should I cheat on this test? Should I give this stranger a ride? Knowing how my selfish and altruistic feelings evolved doesn’t help me decide at all. Most, though not all, moral codes advise me to cultivate altruism. But since the human race has evolved to be capable of a wide range of both selfish and altruistic behavior, there is no reason to say that altruism is superior to selfishness in any biological sense.
In fact, the very idea of an “ought” is foreign to evolutionary theory. It makes no sense for a biologist to say that some particular animal should be more cooperative, much less to claim that an entire species ought to aim for some degree of altruism. If we decide that we should neither “dissolve society” through extreme selfishness, as Wilson puts it, nor become “angelic robots” like ants, we are making an ethical judgment, not a biological one. Likewise, from a biological perspective it has no significance to claim that I should be more generous than I usually am, or that a tyrant ought to be deposed and tried. In short, a purely evolutionary ethics makes ethical discourse meaningless.
Some might draw the self-contradictory conclusion that we ought to drop the word “ought.” I prefer to conclude that ants are anything but human. They may feel pain and pleasure, which are the first glimmerings of purpose, but they’re nowhere near human (much less angelic) goodness. Whether we’re talking about ants, wolves, or naked mole rats, cooperative animal behavior is not human virtue. Any understanding of human good and evil has to deal with phenomena that biology ignores or tries to explain away — such as decency, self-respect, integrity, honor, loyalty or justice. These matters are debatable and uncertain — maybe permanently so. But that’s a far cry from being meaningless.

Q. Identify the option that does not represent a tone or attitude maintained by the author of the passage.

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 4

In the given case, option 3 is the odd one out.

Subjective refers to a tone where the author expresses his ideas and thoughts, something the author does.

Insightful refers to a tone where the author exhibits knowledge and perceptiveness.

Objective refers to a tone where the author only sticks to the facts. This is something that the author does not do.

Critical refers to a tendency to find and call attention to errors and flaws (this is again something that the author does by pointing to the flaws in evolutionary ethics).

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 5

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Leading innovation is not about creating a vision, and inspiring others to execute it. But what do we mean by innovation? An innovation is anything that is both new and useful. Many of you have seen a Pixar movie, but very few of you would recognize Ed Catmull, the founder and CEO of Pixar. It took Ed and his colleagues nearly 20 years to create the first full-length C.G. movie. In the 20 years hence, they've produced 14 movies. When many of us think about innovation, though, we think about an Einstein having an 'Aha!' moment. But innovation is not about solo genius, it's about collective genius. To make a Pixar movie takes about 250 people four to five years.
What we know is, at the heart of innovation is a paradox. You have to unleash the talents and passions of many people and you have to harness them into a work that is actually useful. Innovative organizations are communities that have three capabilities: creative abrasion, creative agility and creative resolution.
Creative abrasion is about being able to create a marketplace of ideas through debate and discourse. Individuals in innovative organizations learn how to inquire, they learn how to actively listen, they also learn how to advocate for their point of view.
Creative agility is about being able to test and refine that portfolio of ideas through quick pursuit, reflection and adjustment. It's about discovery-driven learning where you act, as opposed to plan, your way to the future. It's about running a series of experiments, and not a series of pilots. Experiments are usually about learning. When you get a negative outcome, you're still really learning. Pilots are often about being right. When they don't work, someone or something is to blame.
The final capability is creative resolution. This is about doing decision making in a way that you can actually combine even opposing ideas to reconfigure them in new combinations to produce a solution that is new and useful. When you look at innovative organizations, they never go along to get along. They have developed a rather patient and inclusive decision making process that allows for both/and solutions to arise and not simply either/or solutions.
The infrastructure group of Google is the group that has to keep the website up and running 24/7. When Google was about to introduce Gmail and YouTube, they knew that their data storage system wasn't adequate. Bill Coughran and his leadership team had to figure out what to do about this situation. Instead of creating a group to tackle this task, they decided to allow groups to emerge spontaneously around different alternatives. Two groups coalesced. Big Table proposed that they build on the current system. Build It From Scratch proposed that it was time for a whole new system.
Early on, the teams were encouraged to build prototypes so that they could "bump them up against reality and discover for themselves the strengths and weaknesses of their particular approach." One of the engineers went to Bill and said, "We're all too busy for this inefficient system of running parallel experiments." But as the process unfolded, he began to understand the wisdom He admitted, "If you had forced us to all be on one team, we might have focused on proving who was right, and winning, and not on learning and discovering what was the best answer for Google."
We studied a general counsel in a pharmaceutical company who had to figure out how to get the outside lawyers, 19 competitors, to collaborate and innovate. We also studied Vineet Nayar at HCL Technologies. At HCL technologies the leaders had learned to see their role as setting direction and making sure that no one deviated from it. Vineet inverted the pyramid so that he could unleash the power of the many by loosening the stranglehold of the few.

Q. Why does the author consider the process of innovation paradoxical?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 5

► A paradox is a seemingly absurd or contradictory statement or proposition which when investigated may prove to be well founded or true.

What we know is, at the heart of innovation is a paradox. You have to unleash the talents and passions of many people and you have to harness them into a work that is actually useful.
1 – Contradicts that stand of the article, that innovation is not about vision.

3 – A good leader is a good listener. This option is incorrect.

is a great point of view. It does represent a paradox. But probably will belong to a different article, since it does not touch upon the theme of bottom-up innovation.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 6

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Leading innovation is not about creating a vision, and inspiring others to execute it. But what do we mean by innovation? An innovation is anything that is both new and useful. Many of you have seen a Pixar movie, but very few of you would recognize Ed Catmull, the founder and CEO of Pixar. It took Ed and his colleagues nearly 20 years to create the first full-length C.G. movie. In the 20 years hence, they've produced 14 movies. When many of us think about innovation, though, we think about an Einstein having an 'Aha!' moment. But innovation is not about solo genius, it's about collective genius. To make a Pixar movie takes about 250 people four to five years.
What we know is, at the heart of innovation is a paradox. You have to unleash the talents and passions of many people and you have to harness them into a work that is actually useful. Innovative organizations are communities that have three capabilities: creative abrasion, creative agility and creative resolution.
Creative abrasion is about being able to create a marketplace of ideas through debate and discourse. Individuals in innovative organizations learn how to inquire, they learn how to actively listen, they also learn how to advocate for their point of view.
Creative agility is about being able to test and refine that portfolio of ideas through quick pursuit, reflection and adjustment. It's about discovery-driven learning where you act, as opposed to plan, your way to the future. It's about running a series of experiments, and not a series of pilots. Experiments are usually about learning. When you get a negative outcome, you're still really learning. Pilots are often about being right. When they don't work, someone or something is to blame.
The final capability is creative resolution. This is about doing decision making in a way that you can actually combine even opposing ideas to reconfigure them in new combinations to produce a solution that is new and useful. When you look at innovative organizations, they never go along to get along. They have developed a rather patient and inclusive decision making process that allows for both/and solutions to arise and not simply either/or solutions.
The infrastructure group of Google is the group that has to keep the website up and running 24/7. When Google was about to introduce Gmail and YouTube, they knew that their data storage system wasn't adequate. Bill Coughran and his leadership team had to figure out what to do about this situation. Instead of creating a group to tackle this task, they decided to allow groups to emerge spontaneously around different alternatives. Two groups coalesced. Big Table proposed that they build on the current system. Build It From Scratch proposed that it was time for a whole new system.
Early on, the teams were encouraged to build prototypes so that they could "bump them up against reality and discover for themselves the strengths and weaknesses of their particular approach." One of the engineers went to Bill and said, "We're all too busy for this inefficient system of running parallel experiments." But as the process unfolded, he began to understand the wisdom He admitted, "If you had forced us to all be on one team, we might have focused on proving who was right, and winning, and not on learning and discovering what was the best answer for Google."
We studied a general counsel in a pharmaceutical company who had to figure out how to get the outside lawyers, 19 competitors, to collaborate and innovate. We also studied Vineet Nayar at HCL Technologies. At HCL technologies the leaders had learned to see their role as setting direction and making sure that no one deviated from it. Vineet inverted the pyramid so that he could unleash the power of the many by loosening the stranglehold of the few.

Q. What is the main difference between a pilot and an experiment​

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 6

Experiments are usually about learning. When you get a negative outcome, you're still really learning something that you need to know. Pilots are often about being right.

The other 3 options would have all been true, if we had interchanged the positions of the words pilot and experiment.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 7

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Leading innovation is not about creating a vision, and inspiring others to execute it. But what do we mean by innovation? An innovation is anything that is both new and useful. Many of you have seen a Pixar movie, but very few of you would recognize Ed Catmull, the founder and CEO of Pixar. It took Ed and his colleagues nearly 20 years to create the first full-length C.G. movie. In the 20 years hence, they've produced 14 movies. When many of us think about innovation, though, we think about an Einstein having an 'Aha!' moment. But innovation is not about solo genius, it's about collective genius. To make a Pixar movie takes about 250 people four to five years.
What we know is, at the heart of innovation is a paradox. You have to unleash the talents and passions of many people and you have to harness them into a work that is actually useful. Innovative organizations are communities that have three capabilities: creative abrasion, creative agility and creative resolution.
Creative abrasion is about being able to create a marketplace of ideas through debate and discourse. Individuals in innovative organizations learn how to inquire, they learn how to actively listen, they also learn how to advocate for their point of view.
Creative agility is about being able to test and refine that portfolio of ideas through quick pursuit, reflection and adjustment. It's about discovery-driven learning where you act, as opposed to plan, your way to the future. It's about running a series of experiments, and not a series of pilots. Experiments are usually about learning. When you get a negative outcome, you're still really learning. Pilots are often about being right. When they don't work, someone or something is to blame.
The final capability is creative resolution. This is about doing decision making in a way that you can actually combine even opposing ideas to reconfigure them in new combinations to produce a solution that is new and useful. When you look at innovative organizations, they never go along to get along. They have developed a rather patient and inclusive decision making process that allows for both/and solutions to arise and not simply either/or solutions.
The infrastructure group of Google is the group that has to keep the website up and running 24/7. When Google was about to introduce Gmail and YouTube, they knew that their data storage system wasn't adequate. Bill Coughran and his leadership team had to figure out what to do about this situation. Instead of creating a group to tackle this task, they decided to allow groups to emerge spontaneously around different alternatives. Two groups coalesced. Big Table proposed that they build on the current system. Build It From Scratch proposed that it was time for a whole new system.
Early on, the teams were encouraged to build prototypes so that they could "bump them up against reality and discover for themselves the strengths and weaknesses of their particular approach." One of the engineers went to Bill and said, "We're all too busy for this inefficient system of running parallel experiments." But as the process unfolded, he began to understand the wisdom He admitted, "If you had forced us to all be on one team, we might have focused on proving who was right, and winning, and not on learning and discovering what was the best answer for Google."
We studied a general counsel in a pharmaceutical company who had to figure out how to get the outside lawyers, 19 competitors, to collaborate and innovate. We also studied Vineet Nayar at HCL Technologies. At HCL technologies the leaders had learned to see their role as setting direction and making sure that no one deviated from it. Vineet inverted the pyramid so that he could unleash the power of the many by loosening the stranglehold of the few.

Q. According to the article, what role does vision play in innovation leadership?​

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 7

Refer to passage: Leading innovation is not about creating a vision, and inspiring others to execute it.

The other options also represent viewpoints about vision, but do not sync in with the thoughts this article represents.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 8

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

Leading innovation is not about creating a vision, and inspiring others to execute it. But what do we mean by innovation? An innovation is anything that is both new and useful. Many of you have seen a Pixar movie, but very few of you would recognize Ed Catmull, the founder and CEO of Pixar. It took Ed and his colleagues nearly 20 years to create the first full-length C.G. movie. In the 20 years hence, they've produced 14 movies. When many of us think about innovation, though, we think about an Einstein having an 'Aha!' moment. But innovation is not about solo genius, it's about collective genius. To make a Pixar movie takes about 250 people four to five years.
What we know is, at the heart of innovation is a paradox. You have to unleash the talents and passions of many people and you have to harness them into a work that is actually useful. Innovative organizations are communities that have three capabilities: creative abrasion, creative agility and creative resolution.
Creative abrasion is about being able to create a marketplace of ideas through debate and discourse. Individuals in innovative organizations learn how to inquire, they learn how to actively listen, they also learn how to advocate for their point of view.
Creative agility is about being able to test and refine that portfolio of ideas through quick pursuit, reflection and adjustment. It's about discovery-driven learning where you act, as opposed to plan, your way to the future. It's about running a series of experiments, and not a series of pilots. Experiments are usually about learning. When you get a negative outcome, you're still really learning. Pilots are often about being right. When they don't work, someone or something is to blame.
The final capability is creative resolution. This is about doing decision making in a way that you can actually combine even opposing ideas to reconfigure them in new combinations to produce a solution that is new and useful. When you look at innovative organizations, they never go along to get along. They have developed a rather patient and inclusive decision making process that allows for both/and solutions to arise and not simply either/or solutions.
The infrastructure group of Google is the group that has to keep the website up and running 24/7. When Google was about to introduce Gmail and YouTube, they knew that their data storage system wasn't adequate. Bill Coughran and his leadership team had to figure out what to do about this situation. Instead of creating a group to tackle this task, they decided to allow groups to emerge spontaneously around different alternatives. Two groups coalesced. Big Table proposed that they build on the current system. Build It From Scratch proposed that it was time for a whole new system.
Early on, the teams were encouraged to build prototypes so that they could "bump them up against reality and discover for themselves the strengths and weaknesses of their particular approach." One of the engineers went to Bill and said, "We're all too busy for this inefficient system of running parallel experiments." But as the process unfolded, he began to understand the wisdom He admitted, "If you had forced us to all be on one team, we might have focused on proving who was right, and winning, and not on learning and discovering what was the best answer for Google."
We studied a general counsel in a pharmaceutical company who had to figure out how to get the outside lawyers, 19 competitors, to collaborate and innovate. We also studied Vineet Nayar at HCL Technologies. At HCL technologies the leaders had learned to see their role as setting direction and making sure that no one deviated from it. Vineet inverted the pyramid so that he could unleash the power of the many by loosening the stranglehold of the few.

Q. What best exemplifies the learning-from-your-mistakes approach?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 8

We are looking at two things here. One is a mistake – another is an improvement that follows after the mistake. Pixar is a good case in point. 20 years to make one movie – means that the company must have made a lot of mistakes. 14 movies in the next 20 years – means a significant improvement.

The other options, though about innovation, do not exemplify the learning-from-mistakes approach.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 9

DIRECTIONS for the question : Read the passage and answer the question based on it. 

Melancholy is a word that has fallen out of favor for describing the condition we now call depression. The fact that our language has changed, without the earlier word disappearing completely, indicates that we are still able to make use of both. Like most synonyms, melancholy and depression are not in fact synonymous, but slips of the tongue in a language we’re still learning. We keep trying to specify our experience of mental suffering, but all our new words constellate instead of consolidate meaning. In the essay collection Under the Sign of Saturn, Susan Sontag writes about her intellectual heroes, who all suffer solitude, ill temper, existential distress and creative block. They all breathe black air. According to her diagnostic model, they are all “melancholics.” Sontag doesn’t use the word depression in the company of her role models, but elsewhere she draws what seems like an easy distinction: “Depression is melancholy minus its charms.” But what are the charms of melancholy?
There is a long history in Western thought associating melancholy and genius. We have van Gogh with his severed ear. We have Montaigne confessing, “It was a melancholy humor … which first put into my head this raving concern with writing.” We have Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace. We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering. The charms of melancholy seem to be the charms of van Gogh’s quietly kaleidoscopic color palette: in one self-portrait, every color used on his face is echoed elsewhere in the surroundings. His white bandage complements the canvas in the corner, his yellow skin the wall, his blue hat the blue window. The charms of his work become the charms of his persona and his predicament.
But there’s another kind of portrait possible: the melancholic has not always and everywhere been cast as the romantic hero. In fact, Montaigne’s discussion of melancholy was meant as a kind of Neoplatonic corrective to the old medieval typology of the four humors which cast the “melancholic,” choking on an excess of black bile, as an unfortunate miser and sluggard, despised for his unsociability and general incompetence. That sounds more like it. Indeed, the medieval portrait of melancholy seems to have something in common with our understanding of depression today—or at least of the depressed person we see in pharmaceutical advertisements, whose disease seems to be lack of interest in the family barbecue. We do have our share of romantic geniuses—the suicide of David Foster Wallace is a dark lodestar over recent generations of writers. The pharmacological discourse of depression has not entirely replaced the romantic discourse of melancholy. But on the whole, contemporary American culture seems committed to a final solution.
Both stigmatization and sanctification come with real ethical dangers. On the one hand, there is the danger that hidden in the wish for the elimination of depressive symptoms is a wish for the elimination of other essential attributes of the depressed person—her posture of persistent critique, her intolerance for small talk. On the other hand there is the danger of taking pleasure in the pain of the melancholic, and of adding the expectation of insight to the already oppressive expectations the melancholic likely has for herself. But these ethical dangers are not simply imposed on the unfortunate person from the outside. It is not only the culture at large that oscillates between understanding psychological suffering as a sign of genius and a mark of shame. The language used in both discourses bears a striking resemblance to the language the depressed person uses in her own head.

Q. It can be inferred from the passage that artists such as Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace are attributed to:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 9

In this question, you need to connect the dots. Refer to the portions in bold: Sontag doesn’t use the word depression in the company of her role models, but elsewhere she draws what seems like an easy distinction:

“Depression is melancholy minus its charms.” But what are the charms of melancholy?

There is a long history in Western thought associating melancholy and genius. We have van Gogh with his severed ear. We have Montaigne confessing, “It was a melancholy humor which first put into my head this raving concern with writing.” We have Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace. We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering.

The portions in bold combine to give us option 2 as the correct answer.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 10

DIRECTIONS for the question : Read the passage and answer the question based on it. 

Melancholy is a word that has fallen out of favor for describing the condition we now call depression. The fact that our language has changed, without the earlier word disappearing completely, indicates that we are still able to make use of both. Like most synonyms, melancholy and depression are not in fact synonymous, but slips of the tongue in a language we’re still learning. We keep trying to specify our experience of mental suffering, but all our new words constellate instead of consolidate meaning. In the essay collection Under the Sign of Saturn, Susan Sontag writes about her intellectual heroes, who all suffer solitude, ill temper, existential distress and creative block. They all breathe black air. According to her diagnostic model, they are all “melancholics.” Sontag doesn’t use the word depression in the company of her role models, but elsewhere she draws what seems like an easy distinction: “Depression is melancholy minus its charms.” But what are the charms of melancholy?
There is a long history in Western thought associating melancholy and genius. We have van Gogh with his severed ear. We have Montaigne confessing, “It was a melancholy humor … which first put into my head this raving concern with writing.” We have Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace. We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering. The charms of melancholy seem to be the charms of van Gogh’s quietly kaleidoscopic color palette: in one self-portrait, every color used on his face is echoed elsewhere in the surroundings. His white bandage complements the canvas in the corner, his yellow skin the wall, his blue hat the blue window. The charms of his work become the charms of his persona and his predicament.
But there’s another kind of portrait possible: the melancholic has not always and everywhere been cast as the romantic hero. In fact, Montaigne’s discussion of melancholy was meant as a kind of Neoplatonic corrective to the old medieval typology of the four humors which cast the “melancholic,” choking on an excess of black bile, as an unfortunate miser and sluggard, despised for his unsociability and general incompetence. That sounds more like it. Indeed, the medieval portrait of melancholy seems to have something in common with our understanding of depression today—or at least of the depressed person we see in pharmaceutical advertisements, whose disease seems to be lack of interest in the family barbecue. We do have our share of romantic geniuses—the suicide of David Foster Wallace is a dark lodestar over recent generations of writers. The pharmacological discourse of depression has not entirely replaced the romantic discourse of melancholy. But on the whole, contemporary American culture seems committed to a final solution.
Both stigmatization and sanctification come with real ethical dangers. On the one hand, there is the danger that hidden in the wish for the elimination of depressive symptoms is a wish for the elimination of other essential attributes of the depressed person—her posture of persistent critique, her intolerance for small talk. On the other hand there is the danger of taking pleasure in the pain of the melancholic, and of adding the expectation of insight to the already oppressive expectations the melancholic likely has for herself. But these ethical dangers are not simply imposed on the unfortunate person from the outside. It is not only the culture at large that oscillates between understanding psychological suffering as a sign of genius and a mark of shame. The language used in both discourses bears a striking resemblance to the language the depressed person uses in her own head.

Q. The author of the passage :

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 10

The answer to this question can be derived from the lines: Indeed, the medieval portrait of melancholy seems to have something in common with our understanding of depression today—or at least of the depressed person we see in pharmaceutical advertisements, whose disease seems to be lack of interest in the family barbecue.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 11

DIRECTIONS for the question : Read the passage and answer the question based on it. 

Melancholy is a word that has fallen out of favor for describing the condition we now call depression. The fact that our language has changed, without the earlier word disappearing completely, indicates that we are still able to make use of both. Like most synonyms, melancholy and depression are not in fact synonymous, but slips of the tongue in a language we’re still learning. We keep trying to specify our experience of mental suffering, but all our new words constellate instead of consolidate meaning. In the essay collection Under the Sign of Saturn, Susan Sontag writes about her intellectual heroes, who all suffer solitude, ill temper, existential distress and creative block. They all breathe black air. According to her diagnostic model, they are all “melancholics.” Sontag doesn’t use the word depression in the company of her role models, but elsewhere she draws what seems like an easy distinction: “Depression is melancholy minus its charms.” But what are the charms of melancholy?
There is a long history in Western thought associating melancholy and genius. We have van Gogh with his severed ear. We have Montaigne confessing, “It was a melancholy humor … which first put into my head this raving concern with writing.” We have Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace. We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering. The charms of melancholy seem to be the charms of van Gogh’s quietly kaleidoscopic color palette: in one self-portrait, every color used on his face is echoed elsewhere in the surroundings. His white bandage complements the canvas in the corner, his yellow skin the wall, his blue hat the blue window. The charms of his work become the charms of his persona and his predicament.
But there’s another kind of portrait possible: the melancholic has not always and everywhere been cast as the romantic hero. In fact, Montaigne’s discussion of melancholy was meant as a kind of Neoplatonic corrective to the old medieval typology of the four humors which cast the “melancholic,” choking on an excess of black bile, as an unfortunate miser and sluggard, despised for his unsociability and general incompetence. That sounds more like it. Indeed, the medieval portrait of melancholy seems to have something in common with our understanding of depression today—or at least of the depressed person we see in pharmaceutical advertisements, whose disease seems to be lack of interest in the family barbecue. We do have our share of romantic geniuses—the suicide of David Foster Wallace is a dark lodestar over recent generations of writers. The pharmacological discourse of depression has not entirely replaced the romantic discourse of melancholy. But on the whole, contemporary American culture seems committed to a final solution.
Both stigmatization and sanctification come with real ethical dangers. On the one hand, there is the danger that hidden in the wish for the elimination of depressive symptoms is a wish for the elimination of other essential attributes of the depressed person—her posture of persistent critique, her intolerance for small talk. On the other hand there is the danger of taking pleasure in the pain of the melancholic, and of adding the expectation of insight to the already oppressive expectations the melancholic likely has for herself. But these ethical dangers are not simply imposed on the unfortunate person from the outside. It is not only the culture at large that oscillates between understanding psychological suffering as a sign of genius and a mark of shame. The language used in both discourses bears a striking resemblance to the language the depressed person uses in her own head.

Q. The tone of the author of the passage can be identified as:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 11

In the given case, option 4 is the best fit. Let’s take up every option one at a time and understand the meanings of these options.

Conjectural means based primarily on surmise rather than adequate evidence. Fanciful means not based on fact; unreal. This is clearly not relevant in this case.

Illustrative means clarifying by use of examples and figurative means not literal; using figures of speech. Again, this is not valid in the given case.

Objective means undistorted by emotion or personal bias; based on observable phenomena. A factual tone is one where the author uses facts, data, statistics, etc. Again, this is something that the author of the passage does not do.
Let’s look at the words in option 4 now:
Discursive means: (philosophy) proceeding to a conclusion by reason or argument rather than intuition.

Descriptive means: Serving to describe, inform or characterized by description.

These are clearly generic words that fit in the given context and can be used to describe philosophical contexts like these.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 12

DIRECTIONS for the question : Read the passage and answer the question based on it. 

Melancholy is a word that has fallen out of favor for describing the condition we now call depression. The fact that our language has changed, without the earlier word disappearing completely, indicates that we are still able to make use of both. Like most synonyms, melancholy and depression are not in fact synonymous, but slips of the tongue in a language we’re still learning. We keep trying to specify our experience of mental suffering, but all our new words constellate instead of consolidate meaning. In the essay collection Under the Sign of Saturn, Susan Sontag writes about her intellectual heroes, who all suffer solitude, ill temper, existential distress and creative block. They all breathe black air. According to her diagnostic model, they are all “melancholics.” Sontag doesn’t use the word depression in the company of her role models, but elsewhere she draws what seems like an easy distinction: “Depression is melancholy minus its charms.” But what are the charms of melancholy?
There is a long history in Western thought associating melancholy and genius. We have van Gogh with his severed ear. We have Montaigne confessing, “It was a melancholy humor … which first put into my head this raving concern with writing.” We have Nina Simone and Kurt Cobain, Thelonious Monk and David Foster Wallace. We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering. The charms of melancholy seem to be the charms of van Gogh’s quietly kaleidoscopic color palette: in one self-portrait, every color used on his face is echoed elsewhere in the surroundings. His white bandage complements the canvas in the corner, his yellow skin the wall, his blue hat the blue window. The charms of his work become the charms of his persona and his predicament.
But there’s another kind of portrait possible: the melancholic has not always and everywhere been cast as the romantic hero. In fact, Montaigne’s discussion of melancholy was meant as a kind of Neoplatonic corrective to the old medieval typology of the four humors which cast the “melancholic,” choking on an excess of black bile, as an unfortunate miser and sluggard, despised for his unsociability and general incompetence. That sounds more like it. Indeed, the medieval portrait of melancholy seems to have something in common with our understanding of depression today—or at least of the depressed person we see in pharmaceutical advertisements, whose disease seems to be lack of interest in the family barbecue. We do have our share of romantic geniuses—the suicide of David Foster Wallace is a dark lodestar over recent generations of writers. The pharmacological discourse of depression has not entirely replaced the romantic discourse of melancholy. But on the whole, contemporary American culture seems committed to a final solution.
Both stigmatization and sanctification come with real ethical dangers. On the one hand, there is the danger that hidden in the wish for the elimination of depressive symptoms is a wish for the elimination of other essential attributes of the depressed person—her posture of persistent critique, her intolerance for small talk. On the other hand there is the danger of taking pleasure in the pain of the melancholic, and of adding the expectation of insight to the already oppressive expectations the melancholic likely has for herself. But these ethical dangers are not simply imposed on the unfortunate person from the outside. It is not only the culture at large that oscillates between understanding psychological suffering as a sign of genius and a mark of shame. The language used in both discourses bears a striking resemblance to the language the depressed person uses in her own head.

Q. According to the author of the passage and the information given in the passage:
I. We have arrived at a single consolidated dictionary of terms to define mental suffering.
II. Melancholy and depressed are not the same.
III. According to a certain stream of thought, melancholy is the source of artistic creation and endeavor and not the outcome of artistic processes.

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 12

Statement I can be rejected from the following statement: We keep trying to specify our experience of mental suffering, but all our new words constellate instead of consolidate meaning.

Statement II can be derived from the following statement:Like most synonyms, melancholy and depression are not in fact synonymous, but slips of the tongue in a language we’re still learning.

Statement III is the tempting one. Refer to the lines: We have the stubborn conviction that all of these artists produced the work they did not in spite of, but somehow because of, their suffering.

The first part of statement III is correct but the second part of statement III is incorrect. The author does not imply the second part. The ‘in spite of’ does not imply that artistic endeavors cause mental illness.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 13

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

DISMAL may not be the most desirable of modifiers, but economists love it when people call their discipline a science. They consider themselves the most rigorous of social scientists. Yet whereas their peers in the natural sciences can edit genes and spot new planets, economists cannot reliably predict, let alone prevent, recessions or other economic events. Indeed, some claim that economics is based not so much on empirical observation and rational analysis as on ideology.
In October Russell Roberts, a research fellow at Stanford University's Hoover Institution, tweeted that if told an economist's view on one issue, he could confidently predict his or her position on any number of other questions. Prominent bloggers on economics have since furiously defended the profession, citing cases when economists changed their minds in response to new facts, rather than hewing stubbornly to dogma. Adam Ozimek, an economist at Moody's Analytics, pointed to Narayana Kocherlakota, president of the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis from 2009 to 2015, who flipped from hawkishness to dovishness when reality failed to affirm his warnings of a looming surge in inflation. Tyler Cowen, an economist at George Mason, published a list of issues on which his opinion has shifted (he is no longer sure that income from capital is best left untaxed). Paul Krugman, an economist and New York Times columnist, chimed in. He changed his view on the minimum wage after research found that increases up to a certain point reduced employment only marginally (this newspaper had a similar change of heart).
Economists, to be fair, are constrained in ways that many scientists are not. They cannot brew up endless recessions in test tubes to work out what causes what, for instance. Yet the same restriction applies to many hard sciences, too: geologists did not need to recreate the Earth in the lab to get a handle on plate tectonics. The essence of science is agreeing on a shared approach for generating widely accepted knowledge. Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not.
Nor, it seems, does economics. In a paper on macroeconomics published in 2006, Gregory Mankiw of Harvard University declared: 'A new consensus has emerged about the best way to understand economic fluctuations.' But after the financial crisis prompted a wrenching recession, disagreement about the causes and cures raged. 'Schlock economics' was how Robert Lucas, a Nobel-prize-winning economist, described Barack Obama's plan for a big stimulus to revive the American economy. Mr Krugman, another Nobel-winner, reckoned Mr Lucas and his sort were responsible for a 'dark age of macroeconomics'.
As Mr Roberts suggested, economists tend to fall into rival camps defined by distinct beliefs. Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate" how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say" but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes. Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.
That is worrying. Yet is it unusual, compared with other fields? Gunnar Myrdal, yet another Nobel-winning economist, once argued that scientists of all sorts rely on preconceptions. "Questions must be asked before answers can be given," he quipped. A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

Q. According to the information given in the passage:
I. Scientists and economists are similar.
II. Scientists and economists are not similar.
III. Scientists are more accurate than economists.
IV. Scientists are less disputative that economists.

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 13

Statement I clearly goes against the information given in the passage.
Statement II is the opposite of statement I and mirrors the central idea of the passage.
Statement III can be derived from the lines: Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus.
Statement IV can be derived from the lines: A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 14

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

DISMAL may not be the most desirable of modifiers, but economists love it when people call their discipline a science. They consider themselves the most rigorous of social scientists. Yet whereas their peers in the natural sciences can edit genes and spot new planets, economists cannot reliably predict, let alone prevent, recessions or other economic events. Indeed, some claim that economics is based not so much on empirical observation and rational analysis as on ideology.
In October Russell Roberts, a research fellow at Stanford University's Hoover Institution, tweeted that if told an economist's view on one issue, he could confidently predict his or her position on any number of other questions. Prominent bloggers on economics have since furiously defended the profession, citing cases when economists changed their minds in response to new facts, rather than hewing stubbornly to dogma. Adam Ozimek, an economist at Moody's Analytics, pointed to Narayana Kocherlakota, president of the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis from 2009 to 2015, who flipped from hawkishness to dovishness when reality failed to affirm his warnings of a looming surge in inflation. Tyler Cowen, an economist at George Mason, published a list of issues on which his opinion has shifted (he is no longer sure that income from capital is best left untaxed). Paul Krugman, an economist and New York Times columnist, chimed in. He changed his view on the minimum wage after research found that increases up to a certain point reduced employment only marginally (this newspaper had a similar change of heart).
Economists, to be fair, are constrained in ways that many scientists are not. They cannot brew up endless recessions in test tubes to work out what causes what, for instance. Yet the same restriction applies to many hard sciences, too: geologists did not need to recreate the Earth in the lab to get a handle on plate tectonics. The essence of science is agreeing on a shared approach for generating widely accepted knowledge. Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not.
Nor, it seems, does economics. In a paper on macroeconomics published in 2006, Gregory Mankiw of Harvard University declared: 'A new consensus has emerged about the best way to understand economic fluctuations.' But after the financial crisis prompted a wrenching recession, disagreement about the causes and cures raged. 'Schlock economics' was how Robert Lucas, a Nobel-prize-winning economist, described Barack Obama's plan for a big stimulus to revive the American economy. Mr Krugman, another Nobel-winner, reckoned Mr Lucas and his sort were responsible for a 'dark age of macroeconomics'.
As Mr Roberts suggested, economists tend to fall into rival camps defined by distinct beliefs. Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate" how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say" but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes. Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.
That is worrying. Yet is it unusual, compared with other fields? Gunnar Myrdal, yet another Nobel-winning economist, once argued that scientists of all sorts rely on preconceptions. "Questions must be asked before answers can be given," he quipped. A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

Q. Economics is closer to:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 14

The answer can be derived from the lines: Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not. Nor, it seems, does economics.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 15

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

DISMAL may not be the most desirable of modifiers, but economists love it when people call their discipline a science. They consider themselves the most rigorous of social scientists. Yet whereas their peers in the natural sciences can edit genes and spot new planets, economists cannot reliably predict, let alone prevent, recessions or other economic events. Indeed, some claim that economics is based not so much on empirical observation and rational analysis as on ideology.
In October Russell Roberts, a research fellow at Stanford University's Hoover Institution, tweeted that if told an economist's view on one issue, he could confidently predict his or her position on any number of other questions. Prominent bloggers on economics have since furiously defended the profession, citing cases when economists changed their minds in response to new facts, rather than hewing stubbornly to dogma. Adam Ozimek, an economist at Moody's Analytics, pointed to Narayana Kocherlakota, president of the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis from 2009 to 2015, who flipped from hawkishness to dovishness when reality failed to affirm his warnings of a looming surge in inflation. Tyler Cowen, an economist at George Mason, published a list of issues on which his opinion has shifted (he is no longer sure that income from capital is best left untaxed). Paul Krugman, an economist and New York Times columnist, chimed in. He changed his view on the minimum wage after research found that increases up to a certain point reduced employment only marginally (this newspaper had a similar change of heart).
Economists, to be fair, are constrained in ways that many scientists are not. They cannot brew up endless recessions in test tubes to work out what causes what, for instance. Yet the same restriction applies to many hard sciences, too: geologists did not need to recreate the Earth in the lab to get a handle on plate tectonics. The essence of science is agreeing on a shared approach for generating widely accepted knowledge. Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not.
Nor, it seems, does economics. In a paper on macroeconomics published in 2006, Gregory Mankiw of Harvard University declared: 'A new consensus has emerged about the best way to understand economic fluctuations.' But after the financial crisis prompted a wrenching recession, disagreement about the causes and cures raged. 'Schlock economics' was how Robert Lucas, a Nobel-prize-winning economist, described Barack Obama's plan for a big stimulus to revive the American economy. Mr Krugman, another Nobel-winner, reckoned Mr Lucas and his sort were responsible for a 'dark age of macroeconomics'.
As Mr Roberts suggested, economists tend to fall into rival camps defined by distinct beliefs. Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate"how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say"but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes. Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.
That is worrying. Yet is it unusual, compared with other fields? Gunnar Myrdal, yet another Nobel-winning economist, once argued that scientists of all sorts rely on preconceptions. "Questions must be asked before answers can be given," he quipped. A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

Q. It can be inferred from the passage that:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 15

The answer to this question can be derived from the lines: Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists'' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate"how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say"but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes.

Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 16

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

DISMAL may not be the most desirable of modifiers, but economists love it when people call their discipline a science. They consider themselves the most rigorous of social scientists. Yet whereas their peers in the natural sciences can edit genes and spot new planets, economists cannot reliably predict, let alone prevent, recessions or other economic events. Indeed, some claim that economics is based not so much on empirical observation and rational analysis as on ideology.
In October Russell Roberts, a research fellow at Stanford University's Hoover Institution, tweeted that if told an economist's view on one issue, he could confidently predict his or her position on any number of other questions. Prominent bloggers on economics have since furiously defended the profession, citing cases when economists changed their minds in response to new facts, rather than hewing stubbornly to dogma. Adam Ozimek, an economist at Moody's Analytics, pointed to Narayana Kocherlakota, president of the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis from 2009 to 2015, who flipped from hawkishness to dovishness when reality failed to affirm his warnings of a looming surge in inflation. Tyler Cowen, an economist at George Mason, published a list of issues on which his opinion has shifted (he is no longer sure that income from capital is best left untaxed). Paul Krugman, an economist and New York Times columnist, chimed in. He changed his view on the minimum wage after research found that increases up to a certain point reduced employment only marginally (this newspaper had a similar change of heart).
Economists, to be fair, are constrained in ways that many scientists are not. They cannot brew up endless recessions in test tubes to work out what causes what, for instance. Yet the same restriction applies to many hard sciences, too: geologists did not need to recreate the Earth in the lab to get a handle on plate tectonics. The essence of science is agreeing on a shared approach for generating widely accepted knowledge. Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not.
Nor, it seems, does economics. In a paper on macroeconomics published in 2006, Gregory Mankiw of Harvard University declared: 'A new consensus has emerged about the best way to understand economic fluctuations.' But after the financial crisis prompted a wrenching recession, disagreement about the causes and cures raged. 'Schlock economics' was how Robert Lucas, a Nobel-prize-winning economist, described Barack Obama's plan for a big stimulus to revive the American economy. Mr Krugman, another Nobel-winner, reckoned Mr Lucas and his sort were responsible for a 'dark age of macroeconomics'.
As Mr Roberts suggested, economists tend to fall into rival camps defined by distinct beliefs. Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate" how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say"but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes. Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.
That is worrying. Yet is it unusual, compared with other fields? Gunnar Myrdal, yet another Nobel-winning economist, once argued that scientists of all sorts rely on preconceptions. "Questions must be asked before answers can be given," he quipped. A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

Q. The tone and attitude of the author of the passage can said to be: 

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 16

In the given passage, the author of the passage does repeatedly point out the flaws of economists. But while doing so, he never openly attacks the subject and tries to maintain a balance on the subject.

This makes option 2 the correct answer in this case. Remember, every time he criticizes economics, he provides a counter as well. This makes option 1 and 3 too strong in nature.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 17

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the passage and answer the question based on it.

DISMAL may not be the most desirable of modifiers, but economists love it when people call their discipline a science. They consider themselves the most rigorous of social scientists. Yet whereas their peers in the natural sciences can edit genes and spot new planets, economists cannot reliably predict, let alone prevent, recessions or other economic events. Indeed, some claim that economics is based not so much on empirical observation and rational analysis as on ideology.
In October Russell Roberts, a research fellow at Stanford University's Hoover Institution, tweeted that if told an economist's view on one issue, he could confidently predict his or her position on any number of other questions. Prominent bloggers on economics have since furiously defended the profession, citing cases when economists changed their minds in response to new facts, rather than hewing stubbornly to dogma. Adam Ozimek, an economist at Moody's Analytics, pointed to Narayana Kocherlakota, president of the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis from 2009 to 2015, who flipped from hawkishness to dovishness when reality failed to affirm his warnings of a looming surge in inflation. Tyler Cowen, an economist at George Mason, published a list of issues on which his opinion has shifted (he is no longer sure that income from capital is best left untaxed). Paul Krugman, an economist and New York Times columnist, chimed in. He changed his view on the minimum wage after research found that increases up to a certain point reduced employment only marginally (this newspaper had a similar change of heart).
Economists, to be fair, are constrained in ways that many scientists are not. They cannot brew up endless recessions in test tubes to work out what causes what, for instance. Yet the same restriction applies to many hard sciences, too: geologists did not need to recreate the Earth in the lab to get a handle on plate tectonics. The essence of science is agreeing on a shared approach for generating widely accepted knowledge. Science, wrote Paul Romer, an economist, in a paper published last year, leads to broad consensus. Politics does not.
Nor, it seems, does economics. In a paper on macroeconomics published in 2006, Gregory Mankiw of Harvard University declared: 'A new consensus has emerged about the best way to understand economic fluctuations.' But after the financial crisis prompted a wrenching recession, disagreement about the causes and cures raged. 'Schlock economics' was how Robert Lucas, a Nobel-prize-winning economist, described Barack Obama's plan for a big stimulus to revive the American economy. Mr Krugman, another Nobel-winner, reckoned Mr Lucas and his sort were responsible for a 'dark age of macroeconomics'.
As Mr Roberts suggested, economists tend to fall into rival camps defined by distinct beliefs. Anthony Randazzo of the Reason Foundation, a libertarian think-tank, and Jonathan Haidt of New York University recently asked a group of academic economists both moral questions (is it fairer to divide resources equally, or according to effort?) and questions about economics. They found a high correlation between the economists' views on ethics and on economics. The correlation was not limited to matters of debate"how much governments should intervene to reduce inequality, say"but also encompassed more empirical questions, such as how fiscal austerity affects economies on the ropes. Another study found that, in supposedly empirical research, right-leaning economists discerned more economically damaging effects from increases in taxes than left-leaning ones.
That is worrying. Yet is it unusual, compared with other fields? Gunnar Myrdal, yet another Nobel-winning economist, once argued that scientists of all sorts rely on preconceptions. "Questions must be asked before answers can be given," he quipped. A survey conducted in 2003 among practitioners of six social sciences found that economics was no more political than the other fields, just more finely balanced ideologically: left-leaning economists outnumbered right-leaning ones by three to one, compared with a ratio of 30:1 in anthropology.

Q. A suitable title for the passage is:

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 17

Let''s explore the meanings of all the idioms given in the options:

All sizzle and no steak: A thing or person which fails to measure up to its description or advanced promotion.

A chilles heel: This expression refers to a vulnerable area or a weak spot, in an otherwise strong situation, that could cause one''s downfall or failure.

All in your head: If something is all in your head, it is not real.  It is in your imagination.

All at sea: confused and not certain what to do.

We can clearly see that option 4 is the best answer here.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 18

DIRECTIONS for the question: Identify the most appropriate summary for the paragraph.

Pretentiousness is always someone else's crime. It's never a felony in the first person. You might cop to the odd personality flaw; the occasional pirouette of self-deprecation is nothing if not good manners. Most likely one of those imperfections nobody minds owning up to, something that looks charming in the right circumstances. Being absent-minded. A bad dancer. Partial to a large gin after work. But being pretentious? That's premier-league obnoxious, the team-mate of arrogance, condescension, careerism and pomposity. Pretension brunches with fraudulence and snobbery, and shops for baubles with the pseudo and the vacuous. Whatever it is you do, I'll bet you'd never think it pretentious. That's because you do it, and pretension never self-identifies. Pretentiousness happens over there. In the way he writes. In her music taste. In the way they dress. And who hasn't before described a person, place or thing as pretentious?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 18

This is a tough question and one that you should avoid to solve in the first attempt. The language of the question stem as well as the options will pose a severe challenge while attempting this question under time pressure. On close observation, you will see that option 3 is the only that comes close to the main idea of the paragraph.

In this paragraph, the last three lines are pivotal to understand the paragraph meaning:
Pretentiousness is always someone else's crime. It's never a felony in the first person. Pretentiousness happens over there. In the way he writes. In her music taste. In the way they dress. And who hasn't before described a person, place or thing as pretentious?

In this case, the author wishes to communicate that pretentiousness is something that we allege is present in others but we don't acknowledge our own.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 19

DIRECTIONS for the question: Five sentences related to a topic are given below. Four of them can be put together to form a meaningful and coherent short paragraph. Identify the odd one out. Choose its number as your answer and key it in.

1. Wait, though. Rub your eyes, refocus your gaze, and really, is there any real reason why this ought to be weird?

2. Earlier this year, the 17-year-old son of Will Smith and Jada  Pinkett Smith, brother of Willow, appeared in a Louis Vuitton womenswear campaign.

3. If you wanted to choose a celebrity avatar for everything supposedly weird about The Youth, you could do worse than Jaden Smith: a gnomic tweeter, sometime crystal devotee, self-described "Future of Music, Photography, and Filmmaking," who has little attachment to the gender binary.

4. Jaden Smith, quasar of contemporary teen behaviors, wears a fringed white top and an embellished, knee-length black skirt.

5. The impulse to re examine assumptions has had practical consequences " gender-neutral college dorms and high-school bathrooms " and cultural ripples.


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 19

Statements 3-2-4-1 form the set of connected statements. These four statements are based on the common subject of Jaden Smith.

Only statement 5 is the odd one out here as it is not directly based on this subject.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 20

DIRECTIONS for question: Four sentences related to a topic are given below. Three of them can be put together to form a meaningful and coherent short paragraph. Identify the odd one out. Choose its number as your answer and key it in.

1. Tensions were brewing within the Gulf Cooperation Council for the past six years ever since Qatar started actively supporting the Muslim Brotherhood, a political Islamist movement that the Saudis and their close allies see as a threat to stability in West Asia.

2. The countries said they would halt all land, air and sea traffic with Qatar, eject its diplomats and order Qatari citizens to leave all the Gulf states within 14 days.

3. Saudi Arabia blames Qatar for “harbouring a multitude of terrorist and sectarian groups that aim to create instability in the region”. But such allegations can be raised against most Gulf countries.

4. The dramatic decision by Saudi Arabia, the United Arab Emirates, Bahrain, Egypt and Yemen to suspend diplomatic ties with Qatar could have far-reaching economic and geopolitical consequences.


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 20

4 will be the introductory sentence as it mentions what has been done by some countries  (suspending the ties with Qatar (effect)). 

1 tells that why some countries have suspended all the ties with Qatar (cause). 

3 elaborates 1. Thus 413 is a trio.

2 will be the odd one out as it says that the countries would eject Qatar’s diplomats and its citizens from all the gulf states. .

∴ Sentence 2 is the odd one out.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 21

DIRECTIONS for the question: The five sentences (labelled 1,2,3,4, and 5) given in this question, when properly sequenced, form a coherent paragraph. Decide on the proper order for the sentence and key in this sequence of five numbers as your answer.

1. Technology has given people too many choices, and then instantly relieved them of the need to make them.

2. It now turns out that, even in a potentially unlimited digital marketplace, social networks, rankings, recommendation algorithms and the like focus people attentions on just a few items in the same way.

3. Whatever the arena, the biggest crowds will increasingly gravitate towards just a small number of the most popular hits.

4. Until recently that was seen as a natural consequence of the physical limits on production and distribution.

5. The story of mass entertainment in the internet age is a paradox.


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 21

Sentence 3 initiated the discussion on the topic. Then 4 forms a mandatory pair with three due to noun pronoun pair of ''that'' and the fact stated in 3. Sentence 2 carries forward the idea of sentence 4. Sentence 5 brings in an example about the same theory and finally is concluded in 1.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 22

DIRECTIONS for the question: Identify the most appropriate summary for the paragraph.

In a 1994 case, the Supreme Court clarified the issue of transformative use.
Has the material been used to help create something new or merely copied verbatim into another work?
When taking portions of copyrighted work ask yourself the following questions:
Has the material you have taken from the original work been transformed by adding new expression or meaning?
Was value added to the original by creating new information, new aesthetics, new insights, and understandings?

Q. Based on the guidelines above, which of the following would be a summary written in an example form and write the key for most appropriate option?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 22

By juxtaposing the quotes with the photos of endangered trees, Roger has transformed the remarks from their original purpose and used them to create a new insight.

2 – The creation of a Harry Potter encyclopedia was determined to be “slightly transformative” (because it made the Harry Potter terms and lexicons available in one volume), but this transformative quality was not enough to justify a fair use defense in light of the extensive verbatim use of text from the Harry Potter books.

3 - It did not matter whether the photographer had considered making sculptures; what mattered was that a potential market for sculptures of the photograph existed.

4 - The less you take, the more likely that your copying will be excused as a fair use. However, even if you take a small portion of a work, your copying will not be a fair use if the portion taken is the “heart” of the work, which it is in the Rolling Stone song case.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 23

DIRECTIONS for the question: The five sentences (labelled 1,2,3,4, and 5) given in this question, when properly sequenced, form a coherent paragraph. Decide on the proper order for the sentence and key in this sequence of five numbers as your answer.

1. Thus, unless the plastic is specially designed to decompose in the soil, such materials can last a very long time.

2. This depends upon the plastic (polymer) and the environment to which it is exposed.

3. Commercially available plastics (polyolefin like polyethylene, polypropylene, etc.) have been further made resistant to decomposition by means of additional stabilizers like antioxidants.

4. Plastics do decompose, though not fully, over a very long period of time (in average 100 to 500 years).

5. This means that soil microorganisms that can easily attack and decompose things like wood and other formerly living materials cannot break the various kinds of strong bonds that are common to most plastics.


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 23

Theme revolves around plastic and decomposition of it.
Opening sentence - 4 as it is independent and starts the theme.
Pairs- 3 & 1 are pairs. Look at the word” thus” in sentence 1. it is an effect.

Also Sentence 1 uses words like such material; reference to such material is made in sentence 3. Hence 3 & 1 are pairs. Also E follows 1.

5 gives an explanation of soil microorganisms inefficiency to break bonds in most plastics.
2 is the closing sentence.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 24

DIRECTIONS for the question: The four sentences (labelled 1,2,3 and 4) given in this question, when properly sequenced, from a coherent paragraph. Decide on the proper order for the sentence and key in this sequence of four numbers as your answer.

1. They turn and aerate the soil and make passageways for water drainage, playing a vital role in maintaining soil fecundity and health, they truly are, as biologist E. Q. Wilson has pointed out, the little things that run the world.

2. All their materials, even their most deadly chemical weapons, are biodegradable, and when they return to the soil, they supply nutrients, restoring in the process some of those that were taken to support the colony.

3. Just as there is almost no corner of the globe untouched by human presence, there is almost no land habitat, from harsh desert to inner city, untouched by some species of ant.

4. But although they may run the world, they do not overrun it.


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 24

3 is a good introductory sentence – because of the use of ‘ant’.
The ‘their’ of 2 refers to ants of 3.
1, 4 is a pair because of ‘run’ and ‘overrun’.

*Answer can only contain numeric values
CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 25

DIRECTIONS for the question: Five sentences related to a topic are given below. Four of them can be put together to form a meaningful and coherent short paragraph. Identify the odd one out. Choose its number as your answer and key it in.

1.  This, rather than self-discipline or self-control, per se, is what children would benefit from developing.

2.  What counts is the capacity to choose whether and when to persevere, to control oneself, to follow the rules rather than the simple tendency to do these things in every situation.

3. Remarkably, the predictive power of self-control is comparable to that of either general intelligence or family socioeconomic status.

4.  But such a formulation is very different from the uncritical celebration of self-discipline that we find in the field of education and throughout our culture.

5.  It’s not just that self-control isn’t always good; it’s that a lack of self-control isn’t always bad because it may “provide the basis for spontaneity, flexibility, expressions of interpersonal warmth, openness to experience, and creative recognitions.”


Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 25

The given sentences highlight the importance of self -control or self discipline and hence form a group except 3, which is equating this trait to general intelligence or family socio-economic ties.

∴ 3 is odd one out of the given sentences.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 26

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the information given below and answer the question that follows.

All Iconia computers are available with at least one pre-loaded program from each of three categories: Word Processor – F, G, H; Databases - O, P, R; Browsers – T, U, W. When installing these programs, the company ensures that:

  • An equal number of programs from each category is loaded on a computer.
  • T and P cannot both be loaded on the same computer.
  • If R is loaded on a computer, then U must also be loaded on the computer.
  • O must be loaded on a computer if F is loaded on the computer.
  • If P is loaded on a computer, then R is not loaded on the computer.

Q. How many possible combinations of programs can be loaded on to the computer if P is the only database loaded on the computer?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 26

Since P is the only database installed on the computer, we know that 1 word processor and 1 browser must be loaded on the computer.

We know that P and T both cannot be loaded on the same computer.
So, the browser that is loaded could be either U or W.

We now need to load 1 word processor.

If we load F, then we must load O.
Since O is not loaded, we know that F is not loaded.

So, the word processor that is loaded could be either G or H.

Thus, there are 2 × 2 = 4 combinations.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 27

DIRECTIONS for the question: Read the information given below and answer the question that follows.

All Iconia computers are available with at least one pre-loaded program from each of three categories: Word Processor – F, G, H; Databases - O, P, R; Browsers – T, U, W. When installing these programs, the company ensures that:

  • An equal number of programs from each category is loaded on a computer.
  • T and P cannot both be loaded on the same computer.
  • If R is loaded on a computer, then U must also be loaded on the computer.
  • O must be loaded on a computer if F is loaded on the computer.
  • If P is loaded on a computer, then R is not loaded on the computer.

Q. If two browsers are loaded on the computer, which of the following cannot be true?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 27

Since 2 browsers are loaded, 2 data bases must be loaded.
Since P and R both cannot be loaded on a computer, the 2 databases that can be loaded are either (O, P) or (O, R).

► Case I: When (O, P) is loaded, T cannot be loaded and hence W, U are the browsers to be loaded.

Case II: When (O, R) is loaded, U should be loaded and hence either W or T is the browser to be loaded with U i.e. either (W, U) or (T, U).

In all the above possibilities, it can be checked that we do not have the browsers T and W together.

Thus, T and W cannot be the two browsers that are loaded on the computer.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 28

DIRECTIONS for the question: Go through the following graph/information and answer the question that follows.

As a part of the Best City contest, a news channel invited ten eminent personalities - Q, R, S, T, U, V, W, X, Y and Z - and asked each of them to vote for one of the four shortlisted cities - Bangalore, Delhi, Hyderabad and Mumbai - in each of the two categories-most beautiful city and most happening city. The sum of the number of votes obtained by a city in these two categories put together is considered to be the total number of votes for the city. The city with the maximum total number of votes is finally adjudged as the Best City
After the voting, it was found that:

(i) no two cities got the same number of votes in the most beautiful city category and the same was the case in the most happening city category. However, every city got at least one vote in each of the two categories.

(ii) No two cities got the same total number of votes and Hyderabad emerged as the winner of the contest.

(iii) In case of S and T, in each of the two categories, S voted for the same city as T. However, the same cannot be said to be true for any other pair of persons.

(iv) In the most beautiful city category, no other person voted for the city for which R voted and the same was the case in the most happening city category,

(v) "Except V, who voted for Hyderabad in both the categories and Y, who voted for Bangalore in both the categories, no other person voted for the same city in both the categories,

(vi) Q did not vote for Hyderabad in the most beautiful city category,

(vii) U and W voted for the same city in the most happening city category.

(viii) ln the most beautiful city category, only W and X Voted for Mumbai, while S voted for Bangalore.

Q. Which city did Z vote for as the most beautiful city ?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 28

From the given conditions , the following information can be arrived at as shown.MBC and MHC denote the most beautiful city and most happening city categories

From the above table , it can be seen that in MBC category , all the cities other than Delhi have been voted for by at least one participant. ∴ From (iv) it follows that R voted for Delhi.

Of the remaining people Q, U and Z, whose votes are not determined yet in MBC category, Q has not voted for Hyderabad.
⇒ Hyderabad can get at most 2 out of these 3 votes in the MBC category i.e. a total of at most 3 votes.

It is given that Hyderabad has obtained the highest number of total votes and that, in each ategory ., no two cities have the same number of votes and total number of votes is 10.
∴ The number of votes obtained obtained by any city must be 1,2,3 or 4 in either category.

If Hyderabad has to get highest number of votes it must get exactly 7 votes i.e. 4 votes in MHC category. In case Hyderabad has to get fewer than 7 votes and still get the highest number of votes, condition (ii) would be violated
∴ Hyderabad gets 3 votes from U,V and Z in MBC category and 4 votes in MHC category.

From the above, it can be seen that in the MBC category, Bangalore has already obtained 3 votes in MBC category . Since one city has to get 4 votes, that city has to be Bangalore .
∴ Q,S,T,Y vote for Bangalore under MBC.
 Mumbai gets 2 votes in MBC category. Since the total votes have to be distinct for each city in each of the categories, the following should be the distribution of votes.

From the above title, it follows that R voted for Mumbai in the MHC category.

Also, from (v),(iii) and above discussion, neither U  nor Z voted for Hyderabad in the MHC category. Also, they did-not vote for the same city in the MHC category.

From(vii) U and W voted for the same city in the MHC category.
They did not vote for Bangalore under MHC

They must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ X must have voted for Hyderabad under MHC.
∴ Z must have voted for Bangalore under MHC.
∴ S and T voted for Hyderabad under MHC
⇒ Q must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ The final distribution is as follows.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 29

DIRECTIONS for the question: Go through the following graph/information and answer the question that follows.

As a part of the Best City contest, a news channel invited ten eminent personalities - Q, R, S, T, U, V, W, X, Y and Z - and asked each of them to vote for one of the four shortlisted cities - Bangalore, Delhi, Hyderabad and Mumbai - in each of the two categories-most beautiful city and most happening city. The sum of the number of votes obtained by a city in these two categories put together is considered to be the total number of votes for the city. The city with the maximum total number of votes is finally adjudged as the Best City
After the voting, it was found that,

(i) no two cities got the same number of votes in the most beautiful city category and the same was the case in the most happening city category. However, every city got at least one vote in each of the two categories.

(ii) No two cities got the same total number of votes and Hyderabad emerged as the winner of the contest.

(iii) In case of S and T, in each of the two categories, S voted for the same city as T. However, the same cannot be said to be true for any other pair of persons.

(iv) In the most beautiful city category, no other person voted for the city for which R voted and the same was the case in the most happening city category,

(v) "Except V, who voted for Hyderabad in both the categories and Y, who voted for Bangalore in both the categories, no other person voted for the same city in both the categories,

(vi) Q did not vote for Hyderabad in the most beautiful city category,

(vii) U and W voted for the same city in the most happening city category.

(viii) ln the most beautiful city category, only W and X Voted for Mumbai, while S voted for Bangalore.

Q. Which of the following pairs of persons voted for Bangalore as the most beautiful city?​

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 29

From the given conditions , the following information can be arrived at as shown. MBC and MHC denote the most beautiful city and most happening city categories

From the above table , it can be seen that in MBC category , all the cities other than Delhi have been voted for by at least one participant. ∴ From (iv) it follows that R voted for Delhi.

Of the remaining people Q, U and Z, whose votes are not determined yet in MBC category, Q has not voted for Hyderabad.
⇒ Hyderabad can get at most 2 out of these 3 votes in the MBC category i.e. a total of at most 3 votes.

It is given that Hyderabad has obtained the highest number of total votes and that, in each ategory ., no two cities have the same number of votes and total number of votes is 10.
∴ The number of votes obtained obtained by any city must be 1,2,3 or 4 in either category.

If Hyderabad has to get highest number of votes it must get exactly 7 votes i.e. 4 votes in MHC category. In case Hyderabad has to get fewer than 7 votes and still get the highest number of votes, condition (ii) would be violated
∴ Hyderabad gets 3 votes from U,V and Z in MBC category and 4 votes in MHC category.

From the above, it can be seen that in the MBC category, Bangalore has already obtained 3 votes in MBC category . Since one city has to get 4 votes, that city has to be Bangalore .
∴ Q,S,T,Y vote for Bangalore under MBC.
 Mumbai gets 2 votes in MBC category. Since the total votes have to be distinct for each city in each of the categories, the following should be the distribution of votes.

From the above title, it follows that R voted for Mumbai in the MHC category.

Also, from (v),(iii) and above discussion, neither U  nor Z voted for Hyderabad in the MHC category. Also, they did-not vote for the same city in the MHC category.

From(vii) U and W voted for the same city in the MHC category.
They did not vote for Bangalore under MHC
They must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ X must have voted for Hyderabad under MHC.
∴ Z must have voted for Bangalore under MHC.
∴ S and T voted for Hyderabad under MHC
⇒ Q must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ The final distribution is as follows.

CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 30

DIRECTIONS for the question: Go through the following graph/information and answer the question that follows.
As a part of the Best City contest, a news channel invited ten eminent personalities - Q, R, S, T, U, V, W, X, Y and Z - and asked each of them to vote for one of the four shortlisted cities - Bangalore, Delhi, Hyderabad and Mumbai - in each of the two categories-most beautiful city and most happening city. The sum of the number of votes obtained by a city in these two categories put together is considered to be the total number of votes for the city. The city with the maximum total number of votes is finally adjudged as the Best City
After the voting, it was found that,

(i) no two cities got the same number of votes in the most beautiful city category and the same was the case in the most happening city category. However, every city got at least one vote in each of the two categories.

(ii) No two cities got the same total number of votes and Hyderabad emerged as the winner of the contest.

(iii) In case of S and T, in each of the two categories, S voted for the same city as T. However, the same cannot be said to be true for any other pair of persons.

(iv) In the most beautiful city category, no other person voted for the city for which R voted and the same was the case in the most happening city category,

(v) "Except V, who voted for Hyderabad in both the categories and Y, who voted for Bangalore in both the categories, no other person voted for the same city in both the categories,

(vi) Q did not vote for Hyderabad in the most beautiful city category,

(vii) U and W voted for the same city in the most happening city category.

(viii) ln the most beautiful city category, only W and X Voted for Mumbai, while S voted for Bangalore.

Q. Which of the following pairs of persons voted for the same city in the most happening city category?

Detailed Solution for CAT Practice Test - 6 - Question 30

From the given conditions , the following information can be arrived at as shown.MBC and MHC denote the most beautiful city and most happening city categories

From the above table , it can be seen that in MBC category , all the cities other than Delhi have been voted for by at least one participant. ∴ From (iv) it follows that R voted for Delhi.

Of the remaining people Q, U and Z, whose votes are not determined yet in MBC category, Q has not voted for Hyderabad.
⇒ Hyderabad can get at most 2 out of these 3 votes in the MBC category i.e. a total of at most 3 votes.

It is given that Hyderabad has obtained the highest number of total votes and that, in each ategory ., no two cities have the same number of votes and total number of votes is 10.
∴ The number of votes obtained obtained by any city must be 1,2,3 or 4 in either category.

If Hyderabad has to get highest number of votes it must get exactly 7 votes i.e. 4 votes in MHC category. In case Hyderabad has to get fewer than 7 votes and still get the highest number of votes, condition (ii) would be violated
∴ Hyderabad gets 3 votes from U,V and Z in MBC category and 4 votes in MHC category.

From the above, it can be seen that in the MBC category, Bangalore has already obtained 3 votes in MBC category . Since one city has to get 4 votes, that city has to be Bangalore .
∴ Q,S,T,Y vote for Bangalore under MBC.
 Mumbai gets 2 votes in MBC category. Since the total votes have to be distinct for each city in each of the categories, the following should be the distribution of votes.

From the above title, it follows that R voted for Mumbai in the MHC category.

Also, from (v),(iii) and above discussion, neither U  nor Z voted for Hyderabad in the MHC category. Also, they did-not vote for the same city in the MHC category.

From (vii) U and W voted for the same city in the MHC category.
They did not vote for Bangalore under MHC
They must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ X must have voted for Hyderabad under MHC.
∴ Z must have voted for Bangalore under MHC.
∴ S and T voted for Hyderabad under MHC
⇒ Q must have voted for Delhi under MHC
∴ The final distribution is as follows.

View more questions
Information about CAT Practice Test - 6 Page
In this test you can find the Exam questions for CAT Practice Test - 6 solved & explained in the simplest way possible. Besides giving Questions and answers for CAT Practice Test - 6, EduRev gives you an ample number of Online tests for practice

Top Courses for CAT

Download as PDF

Top Courses for CAT