IELTS Exam  >  IELTS Notes  >  Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic  >  Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic PDF Download

Section - 1

Mental Gymnastics

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(A) THE working day has just started at the head office of Barclays Bank in London. Seventeen staff are helping themselves to a buffet breakfast as young psychologist Sebastian Bailey enters the room to begin the morning’s framing session. But this is no ordinary training session. He’s not here to sharpen their finance or management skills. He’s here to exercise their brains.

(B) Today’s workout, organised by a company called the Mind Gym in London, is entitled "having presence". What follows is an intense 90-minute session in which this rather abstract concept is gradually broken down into a concrete set of feelings, mental tricks and behaviours. At one point the bankers are instructed to shut then eyes and visualise themselves filling the room and then the building. They finish up by walking around the room acting out various levels of presence, from low-key to over the top.

(C) It’s easy to poke fun. Yet similar mental workouts are happening in corporate seminar rooms around the globe. The Mind Gym alone offers some 70 different sessions, including ones on mental stamina, creativity for logical thinkers and "zoom learning". Other outfits draw more directly on the exercise analogy, offering "neurobics" courses with names like "brain sets" and "cerebral fitness". Then there are books with titles like Pumping Ions, full of brainteasers that claim to "flex your mind", and software packages offering memory and spatialawareness games.

(D) But whatever the style, the companies’ sales pitch is invariably the same— follow our routines to shape and sculpt your brain or mind, just as you might tone and train your body. And, of course, they nearly all claim that their mental workouts draw on serious scientific research and thinking into how the brain works.

(E) One outfit, Brainergy of Cambridge, Massachusetts (motto: "Because your grey matter matters") puts it like this: "Studies have shown that mental exercise can cause changes in brain anatomy and brain chemistry which promote increased mental efficiency and clarity. The neuroscience is cutting-edge." And on its website, Mind Gym trades on a quote from Susan Greenfield, one of Britain’s best known neuroscientists: "It’s a bit like going to the gym, if you exercise your brain it will grow."

(F) Indeed, die Mind Gym originally planned to hold its sessions in a local health club, until its founders realised where the real money was to be made. Modem companies need flexible, bright thinkers and will seize on anything that claims to create them, especially if it looks like a quick fix backed by science. But are neurobic workouts really backed by science? And do we need them

(G) Nor is there anything remotely high-tech about what Lawrence Katz, coauthor of Keep Your Brain Alive, recommends. Katz, a neurobiologist at Duke University Medical School in North Carolina, argues that just as many of US fail to get enough physical exercise, so we also lack sufficient mental stimulation to keep our brain in trim. Sine we are busy with jobs, family and housework. But most of this activity is repetitive routine. And any leisure time is spent slumped in front of the TV.

(H) So, read a book upside down. Write or brush your teeth with your wrong hand. Feel your way around the room with your eyes shut. Sniff vanilla essence while listening intently to orchestral music. Anything, says Katz, to break your normal mental routine. It will help invigorate your brain, encouraging its cells to make new connections and pump out neuroteophins, substances that feed and sustain brain circuits.

(I) Well, up to a point it will. "What I’m really talking about is brain maintenance rather than bulking up your IQ," Katz adds. Neurobics, in other words, is about letting your brain fulfill its potential. It cannot create super-brains. Can it achieve even that much, though? Certainly the brain is an organ that can adapt to the demands placed on it. Tests on animal brain tissue, for example, have repeatedly shown that electrically stimulating the synapses that connect nerve cells thought to be crucial to learning and reasoning, makes them stronger and more responsive. Brain scans suggest we use a lot more of our grey matter when carrying out new or strange tasks than when we’re doing well-rehearsed ones. Rats raised in bright cages with toys sprout more neural connections than rats raised in bare cages— suggesting perhaps that novelty and variety could be crucial to a developing brain. Katz, And neurologists have proved time and again that people who lose brain cells suddenly during a stroke often sprout new connections to compensate for the loss—especially if they undergo extensive therapy to overcome any paralysis.

(J) Guy Claxton, an educational psychologist at the University of Bristol, dismisses most of the neurological approaches as "neuro-babble". Nevertheless, there are specific mental skills we can learn, he contends. Desirable attributes such as creativity, mental flexibility, and even motivation, are not the fixed faculties that most of US think. They are thought habits that can be learned. The problem, says Claxton, is that most of US never get proper training in these skills. We develop our own private set of mental strategies for tackling tasks and never learn anything explicitly. Worse still, because any learned skill— even driving a car or brushing our teeth-quickly sinks out of consciousness, we can no longer see the very thought habits we’re relying upon. Our mental tools become invisible to US.

(K) Claxton is the academic adviser to the Mind Gym. So not surprisingly, the company espouses his solution-that we must return our thought patterns to a conscious level, becoming aware of the details of how we usually think. Only then can we start to practise better thought patterns, until eventually these become our new habits. Switching metaphors, picture not gym classes, but tennis or football coaching.

(L) In practice, the training can seem quite mundane. For example, in one of the eight different creativity workouts offered by the Mind Gym—entitled "creativity for logical thinkers" one of the mental strategies taught is to make a sensible suggestion, then immediately pose its opposite. So, asked to spend five minutes inventing a new pizza, a group soon comes up with no topping, sweet topping, cold topping, price based on time of day, flat-rate prices and so on.

(M) Bailey agrees that the trick is simple. But it is surprising how few such tricks people have to call upon when they are suddenly asked to be creative: "They tend to just label themselves as uncreative, not realising that there are techniques that every creative person employs." Bailey says the aim is to introduce people to half a dozen or so such strategies in a session so that what at first seems like a dauntingly abstract mental task becomes a set of concrete, learnable behaviours. He admits this is not a short cut to genius. Neurologically, some people do start with quicker circuits or greater handling capacity. However, with the right kind of training he thinks we can dramatically increase how efficiently we use it.

(N) It is hard to prove that the training itself is effective. How do you measure a change in an employee’s creativity levels, or memory skills? But staff certainly report feeling that such classes have opened their eyes. So, neurological boosting or psychological training? At the moment you can pay your money and take your choice. Claxton for one believes there is no reason why schools and universities shouldn’t spend more time teaching basic thinking skills, rather than trying to stuff heads with facts and hoping that effective thought habits are somehow absorbed by osmosis.

Questions 1-5: Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 1 In boxes 1-5 on your answer sheet, write

  • YES: if the statement is true
  • NO: if the statement is false
  • NOT GIVEN: if the information is not given in the passage

Q.1. Mind Gym coach instructed employees to imagine that they are the building.
Q.2. Mind Gym uses the similar marketing theory that is used all round
Q.3. Susan Greenfield is the founder of Mind Gym.
Q.4. All business and industries are using Mind Gym’s session globally.
Q.5. According to Mind Gym, extensive scientific background supports their mental training sessions.

Questions 6-13: Use the information in the passage to match the people (listed A-D) with opinions or deeds below. Write the appropriate letters A-D in boxes 6-13 on your answer sheet.
(a) Guy Claxton
(b) Sebastian Bailey
(c) Susan Greenfield
(d) Lawrence Katz

NB: You may use any letter more than once

Q.6. We do not have enough inspiration to keep our brain fit.
Q.7. The more you exercise your brain like exercise in the gym, the more brain will grow.
Q.8. Exercise can keep your brain health instead of improving someone’s IQ.
Q.9. It is valuable for schools to teach students about creative skills besides basic known knowledge.
Q.10. We can develop new neuron connections when we lose old connections via certain treatment.
Q.11. People usually mark themselves as not creative before figuring out there are approaches for each person.
Q.12. An instructor in Mind Gym who guided the employees to exercise.

Q.13. Majority of people don’t have appropriate skills-training for brain.

Section - 2

Finding Our WayReading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(A) "Drive 200 yards, and then turn right,” says the car’s computer voice. You relax in the driver’s seat, follow the directions and reach your destination without error. It’s certainly nice to have the Global Positioning System (GPS) to direct you to within a few yards of your goal. Yet if the satellite service’s digital maps become even slightly outdated, you can become lost. Then you have to rely on the ancient human skill of navigating in three-dimensional space. Luckily, your biological finder has an important advantage over GPS: it does not go awry if only one part of the guidance system goes wrong, because it works in various ways. You can ask questions of people on the sidewalk. Or follow a street that looks familiar. Or rely on a navigational rubric: "If I keep the East River on my left, I will eventually cross 34th Street." The human positioning system is flexible and capable of learning. Anyone who knows the way from point A to point B—and from A to C—can probably figure out how to get from B to c, too.

(B) But how does this complex cognitive system really work? Researchers are looking at several strategies people use to orient themselves in space: guidance, path integration and route following. We may use all three or combinations thereof. And as experts learn more about these navigational skills, they are making the case that our abilities may underlie our powers of memory and logical thinking. Grand Central, Please Imagine that you have arrived in a place you have never visited-New York City. You get off the train at Grand Central Terminal in midtown Manhattan. You have a few hours to explore before you must return for your ride home. You head uptown to see popular spots you have been told about: Rockefeller Center, Central Park, the Metropolitan Museum of Art. You meander in and out of shops along the way. Suddenly, it is time to get back to the station. But how?

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(C) If you ask passersby for help, most likely you will receive information in many different forms. A person who orients herself by a prominent landmark would gesture southward: "Look down there. See the tall, broad MetLife Building? Head for that “the station is right below it." Neurologists call this navigational approach "guidance," meaning that a landmark visible from a distance serves as the marker for one’s destination.

(D) Another city dweller might say: "What places do you remember passing? ... Okay. Go toward the end of Central Park, then walk down to St. Patrick’s Cathedral. A few more blocks, and Grand Central will be off to your left." In this case, you are pointed toward the most recent place you recall, and you aim for it.

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

Once there you head for the next notable place and so on, retracing your path. Your brain is adding together the individual legs of your trek into a cumulative progress report. Researchers call this strategy "path integration." Many animals rely primarily on path integration to get around, including insects, spiders, crabs and rodents. The desert ants of the genus Cataglyphis employ this method to return from foraging as far as 100 yards away. They note the general direction they came from and retrace then steps, using the polarization of sunlight to orient themselves even under overcast skies. On their way back they are faithful to this inner homing vector. Even when a scientist picks up an ant and puts it in a totally different spot, the insect stubbornly proceeds in the originally determined direction until it has gone "back" all of the distance it wandered from its nest. Only then does the ant realize it has not succeeded, and it begins to walk in successively larger loops to find its way home.

(E) Whether it is trying to get back to the anthill or the train station, any animal  using path integration must keep track of its own movements so it knows, while returning, which segments it has already completed. As you move, your brain gathers data from your environment—sights, sounds, smells, lighting, muscle contractions, a sense of time passing—to determine which way your body has gone. The church spire, the sizzling sausages on that vendor’s grill, the open courtyard, and the train station—all represent snapshots of memorable junctures during your journey.

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(F) In addition to guidance and path integration, we use a third method for finding our way. An office worker you approach for help on a Manhattan street comer might say: "Walk straight down Fifth, turn left on 47th, turn right on Park, go through the walkway under the Helmsley Building, then cross the street to the MetLife Building into Grand Central." This strategy, called route following, uses landmarks such as buildings and street names, plus directions-straight, turn, go through—for reaching intermediate points. Route following is more precise than guidance or path integration, but if you forget the details and take a wrong turn, the only way to recover is to backtrack until you reach a familiar spot, because you do not know the general direction or have a reference landmark for your goal. The route-following navigation strategy truly challenges the brain. We have to keep all the landmarks and intermediate directions in our head. It is the most detailed and therefore most reliable method, but it can be undone by routine memory lapses. With path integration, our cognitive memory is less burdened; it has to deal with only a few general instructions and the homing vector. Path integration works because it relies most fundamentally on our knowledge of our body’s general direction of movement, and we always have access to these inputs. Nevertheless, people often choose to give route-following directions, in part because saying "Go straight that way!" just does not work in our complex, man-made surroundings.

(G) Road Map or Metaphor? On your next visit to Manhattan you will rely on your memory to get around. Most likely you will use guidance, path integration and route following in various combinations. But how exactly do these constructs deliver concrete directions? Do we humans have, as an image of the real world, a kind of road map in our heads—with symbols for cities, train stations and churches; thick lines for highways; narrow lines for local streets? Neurobiologists and cognitive psychologists do call the portion of our memory that controls navigation a "cognitive map." The map metaphor is obviously seductive: maps are the easiest way to present geographic information for convenient visual inspection. In many cultures, maps were developed before writing, and today they are used in almost every society. It is even possible that maps derive from a universal way in which our spatial-memory networks are wired.

(H) Yet the notion of a literal map in our heads may be misleading; a growing body of research implies that the cognitive map is mostly a metaphor. It may be more like a hierarchical structure of relationships. To get back to Grand Central, you first envision the large scale-that is, you visualize the general direction of the station. Within that system you then imagine the route to the last place you remember. After that, you observe your nearby surroundings to pick out a recognizable storefront or street comer that will send you toward that place. In this hierarchical, or nested, scheme, positions and distances are relative, in contrast with a road map, where the same information is shown in a geometrically precise scale.

Questions 14-18: Use the information in the passage to match the category of each navigation method (listed A-C) with correct statement. Write the appropriate letters A-C in boxes 14-18 on your answer sheet.
NB: you may use any letter more than once
(a) Guidance
(b) Path integration,
(c) Route following    

Q.14. Using basic direction from starting point and light intensity to move on.
Q.15. Using combination of place and direction heading for destination.
Q.16. Using an iconic building near your destination as orientation.
Q.17. Using a retrace method from a known place if a mistake happens.
Q.18. Using a passed spot as reference for a new integration.

Questions 19-21: Choose the correct letter, A, B, c or D. Write your answers in boxes 19-21 on your answer sheet.
Q.19. What does the ant of Cataglyphis respond if it has been taken to another location according to the passage?
(a) Changes the orientation sensors improvingly
(b) Releases biological scent for help from others
(c) Continues to move by the original orientation
(d) Totally gets lost once disturbed 

Q.20. Which of the followings is true about "cognitive map" in this passage?
(a) There is not obvious difference contrast by real map
(b) It exists in our head and is always correct
(c) It only exists under some cultures
(d) It was managed by brain memory 

Q.21. Which of following description of way findings correctly reflects the function of cognitive map?
(a) It visualises a virtual route in a large scope
(b) It reproduces an exact details of every landmark
(c) Observation plays a more important role
(d) Store or supermarket is a must in file map

Questions 22-26: Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 2? In boxes 22-26 on your answer sheet.

  • TRUE if the statement is true
  • FALSE if the statement is false
  • NOT GIVEN if the information is not given in the passage

Q.22. Biological navigation has a state of flexibility.
Q.23. You will always receive good reaction when you ask direction.
Q.24. When someone follows a route, he or she collects comprehensive perceptional information in mind on the way.
Q.25. Path integration requires more thought from brain compared with route following.

Q.26. In a familiar surrounding, an exact map of where you are will automatically emerge in your head.

Section - 3

Mystery in Easter
Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(A) One of the world’s most famous yet least visited archaeological sites, Easter Island is a small, hilly, now treeless island of volcanic origin. Located in the Pacific Ocean at 27 degrees south of the equator and some 2200 miles (3600 kilometers) off the coast of Chile, it is considered to be the world’s most remote inhabited island. The island is, technically speaking, a single massive volcano rising over ten thousand feet from the Pacific Ocean floor. The island received its most well-known current name, Easter Island, from the Dutch sea captain Jacob Roggeveen who became the first European to visit Easter Sunday, April 5,1722.

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(B) In the early 1950s, the Norwegian explorer Thor Heyerdahl popularized the idea that the island had been originally settled by advanced societies of Indians from the coast of South America. Extensive archaeological, ethnographic and linguistic research has conclusively shown this hypothesis to be inaccurate. It is now recognized that the original inhabitants of Easter Island are of Polynesian stock (DNA extracts from skeletons have confirmed this, that they most probably came from the Marquesas or Society islands, and that they arrived as early as 318 AD (carbon dating of reeds from a grave confirms this). At the time of their arrival, much of the island was forested, was teeming with land birds, and was perhaps the most productive breeding site for seabirds in the Polynesia region. Because of the plentiful bird, fish and plant ’ food sources, the human population grew and gave rise to a rich religious and artistic culture.

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

(C) That culture’s most famous features are its enormous stone statues called moai, at least 288 of which once stood upon massive stone platforms called ahu. There are some 250 of these ahu platforms spaced approximately one half mile apart and creating an almost unbroken line around the perimeter of the island. Another 600 moai statues, in various stages of completion, are scattered around the island, either in quarries or along ancient roads between the quarries and the coastal areas where the statues were most often erected. Nearly all the moai are carved from the tough stone of the Rano Raraku volcano. The average statue is 14 feet and 6 inches tall and weighs 14 tons. Some moai were as large as 33 feet and weighed more than 80 tons. Depending upon the size of the statues, it has been estimated that between 50 and 150 people were needed to drag them across the countryside on sleds and rollers made from the island’s trees.

(D) Scholars are unable to definitively explain the function and use of the moai statues. It is assumed that their carving and erection derived from an idea rooted in similar practices found elsewhere in Polynesia but which evolved in a unique way on Easter Island. Archaeological and iconographic analysis indicates that the statue cult was based on an ideology of male, lineage-based authority incorporating anthropomorphic symbolism. The statues were thus symbols of authority and power, both religious and political. But they were not only symbols. To the people who erected and used them, they were actual repositories of sacred spirit. Carved stone and wooden objects in ancient Polynesian religions, when properly fashioned and ritually prepared, were believed to be charged by a magical spiritual essence called mana. The ahu platforms of Easter Island were the sanctuaries of the people, and the moai statues were the ritually charged sacred objects of those sanctuaries.

(E) Besides its more well-known name, Easter Island is also known as Te-Pito-O-Te-Henua, meaning ’The Navel of the World’, and as Mata-Ki-Te-Rani, meaning ’ Eyes Looking at Heaven ’. These ancient name and a host of mythological details ignored by mainstream archaeologists, point to the possibility that the remote island may once have been a geodetic marker and the site of an astronomical observatory of a long forgotten civilization. In his book. Heaven’s Mirror, Graham Hancock suggests that Easter Island may once have been a significant scientific outpost of this antediluvian civilization and that its location had extreme importance in a planet-spanning, mathematically precise grid of sacred sites. Two other alternative scholars, Christopher Knight and Robert Lomas, have extensively studied the location and possible function of these geodetic markers. In their fascinating book, Uriel’s Machine, they suggest that one purpose of the geodetic markers was as part of global network of sophisticated astronomical observatories dedicated to predicting and preparing for future commentary impacts and crystal displacement cataclysms.

(F) In the latter years of the 20th century and the first years of the 21st century various writers and scientists have advanced theories regarding the rapid decline of Easter Island’s magnificent civilization around the time of the first European contact. Principal among these theories, and now shown to be inaccurate, is that postulated by Jared Diamond in his book Collapse: How Societies Choose to or Survive. Basically these theories state that a few centuries after Easter Island’s initial colonization the resource needs of the growing population had begun to outpace the island’s capacity to renew itself ecologically. By the 1400s the forests had been entirely cut, the rich ground cover had eroded away, the springs had dried up, and the vast flocks of birds coming to roost on the island had disappeared. With no logs to build canoes for offshore fishing, with depleted bird and wildlife food sources, and with declining crop yields because of the erosion of good soil, the nutritional intake of the people plummeted. First famine, then cannibalism, set in. Because the island could no longer feed the chiefs, bureaucrats and priests who kept the complex society running, the resulting chaos triggered a social and cultural collapse. By 1700 the population dropped to between one-quarter and one-tenth of its former number, and many of the statues were toppled during supposed "clan wars ” of the 1600 and 1700s.

(G) The faulty notions presented in these theories began with the racist assumptions of Thor Heyerdahl and have been perpetuated by writers, such as Jared Diamond, who do not have sufficient archaeological and historical understanding of the actual events which occurred on Easter Island. The real truth regarding the tremendous social devastation which occurred on Easter Island is that it was a direct consequence of the inhumane behavior of many of the first European visitors, particularly the slavers who raped and murdered the islanders, introduced small pox and other diseases, and brutally removed the natives to mainland South America.

You should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 27-40 which are based on Reading Passage 3 below.
List of Headings 
(i) The famous moai
(ii) The status represented symbols of combined purposes
(iii) The ancient spots which indicates scientific application
(iv) The story of the name
(v) Early immigrants, rise and prosperity
(vi) The geology of Easter Island vii The begin of Thor Heyerdahl’s discovery
(viii) The countering explaination to the misconceptions politaically manipulated
(ix) Symbols of authority and power
(x) The Navel of the World
(xi) The norweigian Invaders’legacy

The reading passage has seven paragraphs, A-G Choose the correct heading for paragraphs A-G from the list below. Write the correct number, i-xi, in boxes 27-30 on your answer sheet.
NB: There are more headings than paragraphs, so you will not use them
Q.28. Paragraph D
Q.29. Paragraph E
Q.30. Paragraph G

Questions 31-36: Do the following statements agree with the information given in Reading Passage 3?

  • TRUE if the statement is true
  • FALSE if the statement is false
  • NOT GIVEN if the information is not given in the passage

Q.31. The first inhabitants of Easter Island are Polynesian, from the Marquesas or Society islands.
Q.32. Construction of some moai statues on the island was not finished.
Q.33. The Moai can be found not only on Easter Island but also elsewhere in Polynesia.
Q.34. Most archeologists recognised the religious and astronomical functions for an ancient society

Q.35. The structures on Easter Island work as an astronomical outpost for extraterrestrial visitors.
Q.36. The theory that depleted natural resources leading to the fail of Easter Island actual has a distorted perspective.

Questions 37-40: Complete the following summary of the paragraphs of Reading Passage, using NO MORE THAN THREE WORDS from the Reading Passage for each answer. Write your answers in boxes 37-40 on your answer sheet.
Many theories speculated that Easter Island’s fall around the era of the initial European contact. Some say the resources are depleted by a ____37_____; The erroneous theories began with a root of the ____38_____ advanced by some scholars. Early writers did not have adequate ____39_____ understandings to comprehend the true result of _____40______ nature of events on the island. The social devastation was in fact a direct of the first European settlers.

Answer

Section - 1

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

Section - 2

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

Section - 3

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

The document Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic is a part of the IELTS Course Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic.
All you need of IELTS at this link: IELTS
34 docs

Top Courses for IELTS

Explore Courses for IELTS exam

Top Courses for IELTS

Signup for Free!
Signup to see your scores go up within 7 days! Learn & Practice with 1000+ FREE Notes, Videos & Tests.
10M+ students study on EduRev
Related Searches

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

,

Previous Year Questions with Solutions

,

Sample Paper

,

practice quizzes

,

shortcuts and tricks

,

mock tests for examination

,

study material

,

pdf

,

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

,

Viva Questions

,

Reading for IELTS Academic Practice Test- 17 | Reading Practice Tests for IELTS Academic

,

Extra Questions

,

Objective type Questions

,

Free

,

Summary

,

Semester Notes

,

Exam

,

ppt

,

Important questions

,

video lectures

,

MCQs

,

past year papers

;